Lade Inhalt...

The equal opportunity illusion: The effects of prejudice and power on information seeking, employee evaluation, task assignment, and estimates of employee success

©2003 Diplomarbeit 96 Seiten

Zusammenfassung

Inhaltsangabe:Abstract:
This study tested the effects of individual variables (prejudice level) and situational factors (power instructions) on information seeking strategies, employee evaluation, estimation of likely success, and task assignment in an employer – employee, ethnicity relevant experimental design, with subjects always assigned the role of employer and an ostensibly other person (a same gender black individual depicted in a photograph) assigned the role of employee. Subjects (N=60) were categorized into groups that varied on power (exclusive or inclusive leadership instruction) and prejudice (quartile split of MRS scores). Participants were asked to select a subset of questions and tasks from various lists for the ostensibly other subject to answer. Participants at a later point in the experiment rated selected questions and tasks. At the end of the experiment the participants were asked to give a final employee evaluation and estimation of likely success for a future project.
Next to the attempt of replicating generally accepted and expected interrelations of power and prejudice with certain attention (information – seeking) strategies and the use of stereotypes and their effect on evaluation and estimation, one of the main focuses of this study is on the effects of the above variables on behavior (final task assignment).
Consistent with predictions participants with a low prejudice level assigned more valued tasks, focused more on strength of the employee and estimated greater employee success than did high prejudice participants. Also participants with inclusive leadership instructions assigned relatively more skill tests with supporting help and estimated greater
employee success than participants with exclusive leadership instructions. Interaction -
effects across the skills test- information seeking-, employee evaluation-, final task assignment-, and estimated success- variables showed that high prejudiced participants in the exclusive leadership style condition respond in stereotype consistent ways significantly more often than participants in the inclusive leadership condition and low prejudice participants.
Zusammenfassung:
Diese Studie untersuchte den Einfluss individueller (Vorurteilslevel) und situationaler Faktoren (induzierter Machtstatus) auf Strategien der Informationssuche, der Bewertung eines Bewerbers in einer Bewerbungssituation, der Einschätzung von Erfolgschancen, sowie der Aufgabenverteilung. Das […]

Leseprobe

Inhaltsverzeichnis


ID 8380
Heidenreich, Swen: The equal opportunity illusion: The effects of prejudice and power on
information seeking, employee evaluation, task assignment, and estimates of employee
success
Hamburg: Diplomica GmbH, 2004
Zugl.: Philipps-Universität Marburg, Diplomarbeit, 1931
Dieses Werk ist urheberrechtlich geschützt. Die dadurch begründeten Rechte,
insbesondere die der Übersetzung, des Nachdrucks, des Vortrags, der Entnahme von
Abbildungen und Tabellen, der Funksendung, der Mikroverfilmung oder der
Vervielfältigung auf anderen Wegen und der Speicherung in Datenverarbeitungsanlagen,
bleiben, auch bei nur auszugsweiser Verwertung, vorbehalten. Eine Vervielfältigung
dieses Werkes oder von Teilen dieses Werkes ist auch im Einzelfall nur in den Grenzen
der gesetzlichen Bestimmungen des Urheberrechtsgesetzes der Bundesrepublik
Deutschland in der jeweils geltenden Fassung zulässig. Sie ist grundsätzlich
vergütungspflichtig. Zuwiderhandlungen unterliegen den Strafbestimmungen des
Urheberrechtes.
Die Wiedergabe von Gebrauchsnamen, Handelsnamen, Warenbezeichnungen usw. in
diesem Werk berechtigt auch ohne besondere Kennzeichnung nicht zu der Annahme,
dass solche Namen im Sinne der Warenzeichen- und Markenschutz-Gesetzgebung als frei
zu betrachten wären und daher von jedermann benutzt werden dürften.
Die Informationen in diesem Werk wurden mit Sorgfalt erarbeitet. Dennoch können
Fehler nicht vollständig ausgeschlossen werden, und die Diplomarbeiten Agentur, die
Autoren oder Übersetzer übernehmen keine juristische Verantwortung oder irgendeine
Haftung für evtl. verbliebene fehlerhafte Angaben und deren Folgen.
Diplomica GmbH
http://www.diplom.de, Hamburg 2004
Printed in Germany

Equal Opportunity Illusion
2
Table of Contents
Acknowledgements... 3
Abstract ... 4
Introduction... 5
Method... 33
Results... 45
Discussion... 48
References... 57
Author's Note... 64
Tables...
Table 1. The effects of participant prejudice level and social influence strategy
on selection of leading questions, skills test assignment, final task assignment,
employee evaluations and estimated success.
65
Appendices...
A: Informed Consent Form
B: Leadership questionnaire
C: Background questionnaire
D: Written Introduction
E: Experimental Manipulation: Reward Focused Instruction
F: Experimental Manipulation: Punishment Focused Instruction
G: Reward Structure
H: Interview Questions
I: Minor Task Problem Example1: Stereotypic White
J: Minor Task Problem Example2: Stereotypic Black
K: Final Task Problem Example1: Stereotypic White
L: Final Task Problem Example2: Stereotypic Black
M: Employee Evaluation Sheet
N: Estimation of employee success
66

Equal Opportunity Illusion
3
Acknowledgments
I am especially grateful to Dr. Theresa K. Vescio for her continuous support and help
throughout the completion and revision of this thesis.
Her knowledge, guidance and commitment of time as well as her contribution of ideas and
different points of view helped me gain new insight and inspiration.

Equal Opportunity Illusion
4
Abstract
This study tested the effects of individual variables (prejudice level) and situational factors
(power instructions) on information seeking strategies, employee evaluation, estimation of
likely success, and task assignment in an employer ­ employee, ethnicity relevant
experimental design, with subjects always assigned the role of employer and an ostensibly
other person (a same gender black individual depicted in a photograph) assigned the role of
employee. Subjects (N=60) were categorized into groups that varied on power (exclusive or
inclusive leadership instruction) and prejudice (quartile split of MRS scores). Participants
were asked to select a subset of questions and tasks from various lists for the ostensibly other
subject to answer. Participants at a later point in the experiment rated selected questions and
tasks. At the end of the experiment the participants were asked to give a final employee
evaluation and estimation of likely success for a future project.
Next to the attempt of replicating generally accepted and expected interrelations of
power and prejudice with certain attention (information ­ seeking) strategies and the use of
stereotypes and their effect on evaluation and estimation, one of the main focuses of this
study is on the effects of the above variables on behavior (final task assignment).
Consistent with predictions participants with a low prejudice level assigned more
valued tasks, focused more on strength of the employee and estimated greater employee
success than did high prejudice participants. Also participants with inclusive leadership
instructions assigned relatively more skill tests with supporting help and estimated greater
employee success than participants with exclusive leadership instructions
.
Interaction -
effects across the skills test- information seeking-, employee evaluation-, final task
assignment-, and estimated success- variables showed that high prejudiced participants in the
exclusive leadership style condition respond in stereotype consistent ways significantly more
often than participants in the inclusive leadership condition and low prejudice participants.

Equal Opportunity Illusion
5
The Equal Opportunity- Illusion: The effects of prejudice and power on information seeking,
employee evaluation, task assignment, and estimates of employee success
The front page of the New York Times on Friday, March 30
th
2001 read "California
Census Confirms Whites Are In Minority ­ State now most diverse". National projections
further indicate that in the United States "persons of color will constitute a numerical
majority sometime between the years 2030 and 2050" (D.W. Sue & D. Sue, 1999, quoting the
U.S. Census Bureau, 1992; D.W. Sue et al., 1997).
Despite the fact that immigration factors and differential birth rates will soon lead to
equivalent numeric representation among members of different ethnic groups in the United
States (D. W. Sue & D. Sue, 1999), there are still large differences in the relative social status
and social power conferred to members of different social groups. Members of dominant
groups (e.g., White men) more often hold high social status, high power positions than do
members of disadvantaged groups (e.g., women, people of color). For example, 95% of all
senior management positions (vice president and above) and 85% of faculty members at
institutions of higher education are held by white males (Sklar, 1999).
Similar differences in social status can be found among ethnic groups in societies
around the world. As Sidanius, Levin, and Pratto (1998) note, most "societies tend to be
stratified along at least one salient social dimension (e.g., along racial, ethnic, tribal,
socioeconomic, religious lines)" and systems of social stratification tend to be remarkably
stable. For example, the Indian caste system has remained intact for over 3,000 years,
whereas White European Americans have dominated and oppressed African-Americans for
hundreds of years (Smith, 1991). More importantly, some social stratification systems have
endured for hundreds of years despite the fact that members of oppressed groups actually

Equal Opportunity Illusion
6
represented a numeric majority (e.g., the Apartheid regime remained in tact in South African
until 1994).
Thus, it seems unlikely that a shift in the numeric representation of various ethnic
groups in the United States will dismantle the well established and longstanding confound
between social group membership and social power. Instead, repeating historical trends, it
seems more likely that the distribution of power will remain stable regardless of the numeric
representation of people from various ethnic groups; Whites will continue to be conferred
power over individuals who belong to disadvantaged social groups.
Given these realities, much recent attention has focused on the relationship between
power and stereotyping. The questions of primary interest in nearly all of this research lies on
who stereotypes whom and why. Consistent with foundational assumptions of the more
general models of impression formation (for instance Fiske & Neuberg, 1990), power and
stereotyping researchers have generally assumed that stereotyping is a default tendency. It
has been assumed that social perceivers process information in the least effortful and/or
cognitively demanding way (e.g., category-based processing) unless motivated to do
otherwise. Building upon this assumption, it has been suggested that low power individuals
should individuate and carefully process information about high power individuals because
outcomes of interest to low power individuals are highly dependent upon high power others.
In contrast, it has been theorized that high power individuals should stereotype low power
others either because they lack the cognitive resources and/or motivation to individuate low
power others (e.g., stereotyping by default) or they do so in order to maintain status
differences (e.g., stereotyping by design; see Goodwin et al., 2000).
Although this line of reasoning still enjoys wide recognition today, the narrowness of
this focus overlooks the social influence dynamics of power relations. While it is true that the
powerful are often less dependent on low power others for valued outcomes, the outcomes of

Equal Opportunity Illusion
7
the powerful are rarely completely independent of low power others. Outcomes for powerful
people often depend on successful interactions with and successful influence of low power
others. For example, a high power manager may want to achieve a certain level of sales
within his department, but can he do so without successfully motivating and influencing the
sales representatives who sell the goods? Likewise, the head of an academic department may
want to produce an internationally visible program, but can she do so without inspiring a high
degree of quality work from the faculty over whom she has power? And if a person in such a
position of power ineffectively or inappropriately exerts power, might such attempts to
influence others actually undermine one's goals?
Taking the above considerations into account one goal of the present research was to
more thoroughly consider the motives and social influence techniques of the powerful and to
consider the possibility that stereotyping is not a default. Instead the present work was
designed to examine the possibility that stereotyping is a consequence of particular kinds of
power situations and results in interactions with some kinds of high power people. To provide
a background for the consideration of this possibility, I first consider the way in which power
has traditionally been conceptualised. I then review the contemporary power and stereotype
literature, both noting the way in which the powerful may perceive and act in stereotypic
ways that maintain the status quo and noting the limitations in the conceptualisations of
power characteristic of most contemporary work.
Conceptualizations of Power
Historically, power has been conceptualised in terms of one person's ability to exert
social influence, or control, over another person's fate (Copeland, 1994; French & Raven,
1959; Imai, 1993; Manz & Gioia, 1983; Thibaut & Kelley, 1959). For example, Weber
(1947) defined power as "the probability that one actor within a social relationship will be in

Equal Opportunity Illusion
8
a position to carry out [his or her] own will despite resistance, regardless of the basis on
which this probability rests ". Importantly, a person does not actually have to influence others
to have power. The potential for person A to influence person B in meaningful psychological
ways, regardless of whether that influence was actually exerted, confers power to person A
(Raven & French, 1959; see also Copeland, 1994). Furthermore, the ability to influence
others is not limited to interpersonal exchanges. One group of people may have power over
another group of people, as Sachdev and Bourhis's (1991) definition of power stresses (i.e.,
the degree of control that one group has over its own fate and the fate of outgroups).
Taking the above mentioned points into consideration I propose the following
definition of power, which adheres to the historical conceptualisation of power as social
influence, but at the same time acknowledges the importance of considering the group vs.
individual as well as the potential vs. actual influence aspects, as mentioned in the above
definitions.
The following definition of power is offered:
"Power is the relative, potential or actual unilateral outcome control of one person or group
over its own or another person's or group's fate in a social relationship".
Power and Stereotyping
Given the traditional and present emphasis on the social influence aspect of power, as well as
social psychologists' long standing interest in the issue of stereotyping and prejudice, much
recent attention has focused on the relationship between power and stereotyping. It has been
suggested that cultural stereotypes and the stereotyping of subordinates justify and maintain
the status quo (e.g. Jost & Banaji, 1994; Sidanius & Pratto, 1999). Furthermore, it has been
proposed that high power people both exhibit stereotype consistent perceptual biases and

Equal Opportunity Illusion
9
behave in ways that make it more likely that they will elicit evidence of stereotype
confirmation from low power others. At a societal level it has also been suggested that
stereotyping is a major contributor to the stratification and maintenance of power differentials
in both modern societies (e.g. European Americans' dominance over African Americans in
the U.S.A.) and ancient societies (e.g. caste system in India). Simply stated, "stereotyping
subordinate group members is the mechanism through which dominant groups maintain the
status quo" (Jost & Banaji, 1994, 1998).
Perceptual and Behavioral Confirmation
There are two means by which the powerful can come to behave in ways that justify
and maintain the status quo. First, high power people may form erroneous and stereotype
consistent impressions of low power others, which has been referred to as perceptual
confirmation. For example, high power (as compared to low power) individuals are perceived
as more fair when using punishment strategies (Molm, Quist and Wiseley, 1994) and
perceived to be more accurate judges of others when, in fact, they are less accurate judges
(Ebenbach and Keltner, 1998). Beyond being more likely to perceive others in a stereotypic
and inaccurate manner, high power people may also behave or interact in ways that elicit
stereotypic behaviors from low power others, which is referred to as behavioral confirmation.
For example, Word, Zanna and Cooper (1974) demonstrated, that when interviewer
participants interacted with a Black confederate interviewee, participants sat further away
from the confederate, conducted shorter interviews, made less eye contact and made more
speech errors as compared to those in the White confederate interviewee condition. In a
second study, confederate interviewers were trained to act either like the participants in the
White confederate or Black confederate conditions of the first study. Trained confederates
then interviewed participants. Demonstrating that the behaviors of the relatively powerful

Equal Opportunity Illusion
10
have meaningful consequences for those low in power, independent raters evaluated the
performance of the interviewee participants. Participants who were treated like the Black
confederate of Study 1 (e.g., less eye contact, more speech errors) were evaluated as
performing more poorly than those treated like the White confederate of Study 1.
Importantly, as power, status, and group number increase, discriminatory behaviors have
been found to increase (Sachdev & Bourhis, 1991). In fact, Operario and Fiske (1998) have
suggested that merely having power, and thus control, motivates bias and the tendency to
stereotype and discriminate against others. According to these authors, "racial oppression
derives from a) power ­ the disproportionate ability of some individuals or groups to control
other people's outcomes; and b) prejudice ­ the universal tendency to favor the in- group
over the out- group. Racism functions additively from asymmetrical power and racial
prejudice" (p.49). Importantly, it has been suggested that this occurs both automatically, as
soon as perceivers can place others within a category, and unintentionally, even among the
most well-intentioned and egalitarian people" (Operario and Fiske, 1998).
The vicious circle of perpetual power and stereotype reinforcement
The preceding consideration might have made one thing already clear: Both
stereotypes (or cognitive representations of outgroups) and prejudice (or one's negative
feelings about outgroups) may initiate a vicious circle in which biased perceptions and biased
feeling reinforce one another. More specifically, sometimes power may enhance the use of
stereotypes, with the latter surfacing in perceptual and/or behavioral biases. At other times
the reliance on stereotypes contributes to a stratification of existing power differentials.
Therefore, on top of one-way effects of power on stereotyping and vice versa, as
described above, we find that power and the two forms of stereotyping (perceptual and
behavioral) may also continuously reinforce each other. Various studies (e.g. Goodwin,

Equal Opportunity Illusion
11
Operario and Fiske, 1998) show that situational control and interpersonal dominance (power)
are conditions that promote motives to stereotype, leading to cognitive and judgment biases
that in turn cumulatively reinforce the status quo and thereby exaggerating preexisting power
differentials.
Given that power has traditionally been defined in dynamic social influence terms and
that there is ample evidence that the powerful perceive and behave toward low power others
in ways that make it likely that low power others will confirm (or be perceived as confirming)
expectations, much recent attention has been directed to attempts that explain the relationship
between power and stereotyping. The most influential model of power and stereotyping has
been Fiske's (1993) power as control (PAC) model, which is reviewed below.
The Power as Control (PAC) Model
In their investigation of the interaction between power and stereotyping, Fiske and her
colleagues argue that the powerless carefully attend to the powerful who control their
outcomes in an effort to enhance prediction and control. As a result, powerless people form
relatively complex and non-stereotypic impressions of others who have control over them. In
contrast, the powerful, pay less attention, and thus are more vulnerable to stereotyping. Fiske
and her colleagues' (e.g., Fiske, 1993; see also Dépret & Fiske, 1999; Goodwin et al., 1998;
Goodwin et al., 2000) model assumes three main reasons for perceivers' increased (as
compared to targets') use of stereotypes. These reasons are: a) high power people have more
demands on their attention (lack of cognitive resources) than do low power people, b) low
power people are more dependent on high power others for outcomes than the reverse, and c)
the kind of people that come to hold high power positions might tend to be high on certain
individual difference variables such as social dominance. Importantly, this theorizing is
mainly based on the assumption that cultural stereotypes are universally known and

Equal Opportunity Illusion
12
internalized, providing a source of the expectations that the powerful bring to bear on a
situation.
Interestingly, this model considers individual factors to be only of minor importance,
exerting merely indirect influence on stereotype use. According to the PAC model, individual
variables, such as social dominance or prejudice, are linked to people in high power
positions, which might have a tendency to be high on such variables, as the theory points out
(see above). Whereas Fiske et al. in their discussion about why a certain group of individuals
(powerful) stereotype others more than another group of individuals (powerless) regard
individual factors only relevant in this indirect way, other authors, e.g. Christiansen, Kaplan
and Jones (1999), do mention such an individual factor, namely prejudice. However, in the
discussion of their findings the authors agree that participants' different prejudice levels were
irrelevant for the way they represent certain groups and categories internally: Findings
showed that evaluations by high-prejudice participants were more negative than those of low-
prejudice participants only when the target applicant was described by a single negative
stereotype. The authors concluded from these findings that both groups had similarly
negative predispositions toward minorities, with those of more prejudiced individuals
requiring less negative stereotypical information to be activated.
The most imperative evidence supporting Fiske's (1993) assumptions seems to be
Devine's (1989) dual process model of racial prejudice. According to Devine's model all
White Americans are aware of and have internalized the dominant cultural stereotype of
African Americans and these stereotypic representations can be activated automatically.
More specifically, according to Devine, this automatic activation happens in both, high and
low prejudice White individuals given the mere presence of a Black individual or mere
activation of the category "Black". This, so the model continues, would lead both high and
low prejudiced perceivers to judge African American individuals in relation to the cultural

Equal Opportunity Illusion
13
stereotypes. Fiske's reasoning relies on this model; Fiske assumes that stereotypes are
accessible to all high power perceivers and those in high power positions are especially prone
to rely on stereotype -based expectations that guide information seeking, behavior and
inferences. While highly influential and having impetus on a great deal of research, the power
as control (PAC) model equates outcome control (or one's ability to have relative greater say
in an outcome) with power. The theoretical statements of the PAC model and tests of that
model omit the dynamic social influence components of power relations, as noted at the
outset. Also problematic are three additional points. First, as noted previously, the theory
assumes that stereotypes are universally internalized, i.e., stereotyping is a default tendency.
Second, hypotheses derived from this model and tests of those hypotheses have focused
simply on the descriptive question of "who stereotypes whom" omitting a consideration of
"why" stereotyping occurs. Thirdly, Fiske's primary tool of measuring stereotyping is
attention. This indirect method by which stereotyping has been measured most predominantly
does constitute a major problem as this focus on attention has lead to a neglect of other
perhaps more suitable variables, like motivation, for instance. More will be said in this regard
later (see "the aspect of motivation in stereotyping", p.20). These three points are problematic
in light of the fact that there is a growing literature showing that not all individuals internalize
the cultural stereotypes and numerous situations have been shown to influence stereotyping. I
will review the relevant literature illustrating the problems with these assumptions in the
sections that follow.
Universally internalized stereotypes?
A common feature to several theories of racial prejudice is the notion that White
Americans internalize the negative cultural stereotype of Black Americans (e.g., Devine,
1989; Gaertner & Dovidio, 1986). Presumably, as a result of shared socialization experiences,

Equal Opportunity Illusion
14
all Whites are aware of the cultural stereotype, which gets represented cognitively and can be
activated automatically. From this perspective the difference between high and low
prejudiced people lies not in the presence or absence of the cognitive representations (all
people have stereotypic representations), but in the conscious control that is exerted in
attempts to control stereotypic responding. It has been suggested that low prejudiced people
"put on the brakes" and try to limit the influences of stereotypes in situations in which they
have the resources and awareness to control stereotypic responding.
While there is no disputing the fact that White Americans tend to be universally
knowledgeable of the cultural stereotypes of Black Americans, recent findings question the
notion that such knowledge is similarly internalized by both high and low prejudiced people
and automatically activated. Additionally, there is a growing body of evidence noting that
situational factors importantly influence whether stereotypic perception and behavior ensues.
All of these findings stand in contrast to the assumption that stereotypes are universally
internalized, thus questioning a fundamental assumption upon which Fiske's (1993) PAC
model was derived.
Individual Differences in stereotyping
While many theories have assumed that all people have cognitive representations of
the dominant cultural stereotype, which is automatically activated in the mere presence of an
African American individual, recent research demonstrates that there are critical differences
in the cognitive representations that high and low prejudiced Whites have of African
Americans. Lepore and Brown (1997), for example, showed that the relations among
categorization, stereotyping, and prejudice are more flexible than is often assumed. It was
shown that differences in responses between high- and low- prejudice people occurred when
a category, rather than stereotypic traits, was subliminally primed. When the category Blacks

Equal Opportunity Illusion
15
was subliminally primed high -prejudice participants formed a more negative and less
positive impression of the target person than did participants in the no prime condition, with
low -prejudice people tending in the opposite direction. But when valenced stereotype
content was primed, high- and low- prejudice people increased negative ratings. These
differences were found despite the fact that the two groups of high-and low prejudice people
share the same knowledge of the stereotypes of Black people, as could be shown in an earlier
study (study 1).
Thus, high-and low-prejudice people differ in their automatic responses to category
activation, which suggests that they hold different representations because of their beliefs and
despite their common stereotype knowledge. This directly opposes the notion of many
authors that most White Americans hold negative stereotypes of Black Americans (e.g.
Gaertner & Dovidio, 1986), which are activated automatically when confronted with a
member of that category, as Devine's (1989) "mere presence" -argument states (see page 12)
or Fiske & Neuberg (1990) conclude.
Taking the above arguments of Lepore and Brown into consideration we can no
longer talk about universally internalized stereotypes, but instead state that high ­ and low-
prejudiced individuals have different stereotypes. According to Vescio and Biernat (1999),
both high and low prejudiced people have internalized representations of group differences in
social status (e.g., blacks lower social status, lower income, less educated), but high and low
prejudice people differ in their explanation of such differences in social status. Whereas low-
prejudiced individuals primarily regard situational components of stereotypes as the cause
(discrimination, prejudice etc. lead to blacks' lower social status), high-prejudiced individuals
believe in a dispositional causation for those differences (e.g., blacks are lazy, unmotivated,
unintelligent...). That is, only high prejudiced people have the traits consistent with the
cultural stereotype. As a result both groups have different expectations, which eventually can

Equal Opportunity Illusion
16
be violated. Supporting evidence for this line of reasoning of differing stereotype content
between high- and low prejudice people comes from various studies. Vescio & Biernat
(1999), for example showed that individual differences in prejudice level affect conditions
under which evaluations of outgroup targets are more extreme and stereotypic. The authors
hypothesized that high prejudiced White perceivers, having internalized the negative cultural
stereotype of Black Americans, would expect a lack of intellect as well as lack of motivation
in Blacks. On the other hand, low prejudiced Whites, rejecting the cultural stereotype, were
hypothesized to expect academic achievement and success. Violations of these expectations
were assumed to lead to more extreme appraisals in such that evaluations become more
extreme in the direction of the expectancy violation. The results showed precisely that. High
prejudiced perceivers evaluated an expectancy violating high-quality lecture given by a Black
professor more extremely favorable (i.e., rated much more favorably than a similar lecture
administered by a White professor). In contrast, low prejudiced Whites evaluated an
expectancy violating poor quality lecture given by a Black professor much more extremely
and in a negative direction.
Another study, supporting the idea of differences between high- and low prejudiced
individuals' representations and explanations of stereotypes was offered by Larsen et al. 1980
(cited by Jones 1997, p.156). In this study, participants who scored high on the Modern
Racism Scale (MRS; McConahay, 1986), unlike low scoring subjects, did not suppress the
effects of stereotypes on their judgments when given a chance to do so. Moreover, when they
received feedback suggesting they were biased toward either black males or gay men, those
with negative attitudes (as measured by the MRS for Blacks or the Heterosexual Attitude
toward Homosexuals [HATH] for gay men) felt no compunction about their biases, but those
low in modern racism or anti-gay attitudes felt emotional distress when made aware of their
biases.

Equal Opportunity Illusion
17
Extending and investigating even further the idea of different stereotype
representations, others have proposed (based on various studies investigating prejudices
against Blacks, homosexuals, and fat people) that low prejudiced people might have such
situational explanations as a result of humanitarian/egalitarian self-concepts, which are
negatively associated with all measures of prejudice and discrimination. This self-concept, so
the authors argue, does serves as a ,,prejudice antidote", as was demonstrated by the
association of Protestant Work Ethic (PWE) values with increased prejudice toward Blacks
homosexuals, and fat people (e.g. Biernat, Vescio, Theno & Crandall, 1996).
The importance of inter-individual differences and their influence on the accessibility
of and the willingness to use stereotypes is further highlighted by social dominance theorists
(e.g., Sidanius & Pratto, 1999) and their proposition of a construct, which is called "Social
Dominance Orientation" (SDO); an individual difference variable, defined as "the degree to
which individuals desire and support group-based hierarchy and the domination of `inferior'
groups by `superior' groups" (p.48). People high in SDO, for example more strongly endorse
racial stereotypes, attributing the lower social status of racial outgroups to characteristics of
individual group members.
Differences between high- and low- prejudiced people appear at an automatic level,
supporting the idea of a flexible link between categorization and stereotyping. "It is
endorsement, not knowledge that is likely to shape the representation in memory,
strengthening the links between the category label and certain stereotypic features instead of
others. Prejudice does not resemble a habit that has to be broken (referring to Devine's
assumption, 1989) but one that is, for some people, already broken" and "it is not knowledge
which strengthens the links between category and stereotype, it is rather the endorsement"
(Lepore and Brown, 1997). The contemporary findings contemplated above, argue against the
universal internalization theory and clearly support the idea that high prejudice persons and

Equal Opportunity Illusion
18
low prejudice persons have different representations of groups. Moreover, this is the case
because of their beliefs and despite their common knowledge about stereotypes!
Situational differences in stereotyping
Historically, there have been various approaches to the understanding the concept of
prejudice (for an overview see Jones, 1997, p.143ff). Social psychology has mainly regarded
prejudice as an individual-level phenomenon. Zanna (1994), for instance proposed four
sources of influence on the development and occurrence of prejudice, all lying within the
individual:
1) Stereotypical beliefs (the notion that typical members of the group possess certain
characteristics or traits),
2) Symbolic beliefs (the notion that typical target group members violate cherished majority-
or dominant-group traditions, customs, and values),
3) Emotions that are aroused by a member or members of the group, and
4) Past experiences with members of the group.
Other theories for explaining prejudice that also stress the individual ­ level of
prejudice are 1) terror management theory (cf. Solomon, Greenberg and Pyszczynski, 1991;
Greenberg, Pyszczynski et al., 1990), which regards prejudice as a terror management
reaction to perceived threat and heightened awareness of one's very own mortality, 2) ego-
defensiveness theory (Dollard & Miller,1941), 3)Adorno at al.'s (1951) authoritarian
personality theory, or 4)Altemeyer's (1993) right wing authoritarian personality theory (in
Jones, 1997, p.145ff), to name a few.
Although, this focus on the individual level is not surprising, conceiving that we
generally speak of "a prejudiced person" or "individuals who are prejudiced", there are, apart

Equal Opportunity Illusion
19
from individual differences, also situational variables, which also have strong effects on
prejudiced behavior.
Thus, an often-neglected fact is that individuals usually do not exist in isolation.
Human beings are social beings, socialized in communities with certain norms and values,
constantly interacting with other individuals. In trying to understand the reasons for and
influences on prejudice, society itself, in providing manifold situations and contexts for
interactions, has to be considered and regarded as a major source of influence on prejudice.
Sidanius and Pratto's social dominance theory (SDT, 1999), for instance, by integrating
models from various fields (see previous page), does take this important aspect into account
and puts much emphasis on the interface of individual and society.
Thus, situational norms, as part of situational factors in general, influence stereotyping,
prejudice and/or discriminatory behavior. Dovidio and Gaertner (2000) for instance,
demonstrated very clearly how ambiguity as a single situational factor might offer an excuse
for aversive racists to rationalize their discriminatory behavior on the basis of some other
factor than race. Ambiguity, as the situational factor in this case, therefore, can be thought of
as a window, facilitating to show individuals' true attitudes and feelings.
Another example is the already mentioned study by Vescio and Biernat (1999), in
which the authors argue that low prejudiced -and high prejudiced individuals' different
stereotype-based expectancies and their respective violations may affect own performance as
well as judgment of a high status target. Situational variables in that study were ethnicity of
the target person (lecturer) and quality of the lecture, which demonstrably affected
perceiver's judgment and performance. In sum, a consideration of situational factors is
important for a deeper understanding of the concept and mechanisms of prejudice.

Equal Opportunity Illusion
20
The aspect of motivation in stereotyping
As pointed out above, when reviewing Fiske's PAC model (see p.11), stereotyping is
normally assessed via perception or behavior. This reliance on attention as the major means
of measuring stereotyping has lead to a neglect of other, possibly more valid assessment
techniques. While attention has been proposed the major measurement of stereotypes, the
aspect of motivation has been completely omitted when talking about the intentional reliance
on stereotypic information (stereotyping by design). As noted above, Fiske and her
colleagues (Fiske, 1993; Dépret &Fiske, 1999; or Goodwin et al., 1998 and Goodwin et al.,
2000) do talk about cognitive load as reasons for powerful individuals' reliance on
stereotypes, but omit the aspect of motivation when attending to some but not other
information.
Other authors have tried to integrate both, attention and motivation variables in their
discussion of stereotyping. Supporting the cognitive load ­ argument as a reason for
stereotyping, Bodenhausen (1993) for instance, sees affect as a possible influence on our
judgment processes in that arousal limits our cognitive capacity and therefore make us more
susceptible to rely on simpler and more dominant processing strategies like stereotypes. On
the other hand, and more in line with the motivational aspect argument, Bodenhausen
hypothesizes, the second way affect influences the use of stereotypes to be by means of
motivational influences. These, so the author argues, affect our willingness to use certain
cognitive capacities and result in more or less use of stereotypes, depending on the emotion
involved.
More support for a stronger inclusion of motivational factors into the discussion of
stereotyping by design, comes from various other studies. Reviewing literature on dissonance
theory (Festinger, 1957), for example, asks for caution in the use of attention as a measure for
stereotyping. Many "selective exposure" experiments (see for example Frey, 1986), have

Details

Seiten
Erscheinungsform
Originalausgabe
Erscheinungsjahr
2003
ISBN (eBook)
9783832483807
ISBN (Paperback)
9783838683805
DOI
10.3239/9783832483807
Dateigröße
1 MB
Sprache
Englisch
Institution / Hochschule
Philipps-Universität Marburg – Psychologie
Erscheinungsdatum
2004 (November)
Note
2,0
Schlagworte
vorurteile rassismus macht diskriminierung stereotypen
Produktsicherheit
Diplom.de
Zurück

Titel: The equal opportunity illusion: The effects of prejudice and power on information seeking, employee evaluation, task assignment, and estimates of employee success
book preview page numper 1
book preview page numper 2
book preview page numper 3
book preview page numper 4
book preview page numper 5
book preview page numper 6
book preview page numper 7
book preview page numper 8
book preview page numper 9
book preview page numper 10
book preview page numper 11
book preview page numper 12
book preview page numper 13
book preview page numper 14
book preview page numper 15
book preview page numper 16
book preview page numper 17
book preview page numper 18
book preview page numper 19
book preview page numper 20
96 Seiten
Cookie-Einstellungen